Why Don't Students Like School?. Daniel T. Willingham. Читать онлайн. Newlib. NEWLIB.NET

Автор: Daniel T. Willingham
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Серия:
Жанр произведения: Учебная литература
Год издания: 0
isbn: 9781119715801
Скачать книгу
very simple model of the mind. On the left of Figure 1.7 is the environment, full of things to see and hear, problems to be solved, and so on. On the right is one component of your mind that scientists call working memory. For the moment, consider it to be synonymous with consciousness; it holds the stuff you're thinking about. The arrow from the environment to working memory shows that working memory is the part of your mind where you are aware of what is around you: the sight of a shaft of light falling onto a dusty table, the sound of a dog barking in the distance, and so forth. Of course you can also be aware of things that are not currently in the environment; for example, you can recall the sound of your mother's voice, even if she's not in the room (or indeed no longer living). Long-term memory is the vast storehouse in which you maintain your factual knowledge of the world: that leopards have spots, that your favorite flavor of ice cream is chocolate, that your three-year-old surprised you yesterday by mentioning kumquats, and so on. Factual knowledge can be abstract; for example, it would include the idea that triangles are closed figures with three sides and your knowledge of what a dog generally looks like. All of the information in long-term memory resides outside of awareness. It lies quietly until it is needed and then enters working memory and so enters consciousness. For example, if I asked you, “What color is a polar bear?” you would say, “white” almost immediately. That information was in long-term memory 30 seconds ago, but you weren't aware of it until I posed the question that made it relevant to ongoing thought, whereupon it entered working memory.

Schematic illustration of a playing board with three pegs. There are three rings of decreasing size on the leftmost peg. The goal is to move all three rings from the leftmost peg to the rightmost peg. There are just two rules about how you can move rings: you can move only one ring at a time, and you can't place a larger ring on top of a smaller ring.

      Source: © Greg Culley.

      Source: © Greg Culley.

      The description of thinking makes it clear that knowing how to combine and rearrange ideas in working memory is essential to successful thinking. For example, in the discs and pegs problem, how do you know where to move the discs? If you hadn't seen the problem before, you probably felt like you were pretty much guessing. You didn't have any information in long-term memory to guide you, as depicted in Figure 1.9. But if you have had experience with this particular type of problem, then you likely have information in long-term memory about how to solve it, even if the information is not foolproof. For example, try to work this math problem in your head:

equation

      You know just what to do for this problem. The sequence of your mental processes was likely something close to this:

      1 Multiply 8 and 7.

      2 Retrieve the fact that 8 × 7 = 56 from long-term memory.

      3 Remember that the 6 is part of the solution, then carry the 5.

      4 Multiply 7 and 1.

      5 Retrieve the fact that 7 × 1 = 7 from long-term memory.

      6 Add the carried 5 to the 7.

      7 Retrieve the fact that 5 + 7 = 12 from long-term memory.

      8 Put the 12 down, append the 6.

      9 The answer is 126.

      Your long-term memory contains not only factual information, such as the color of polar bears and the value of 8 × 7, but it also contains what we'll call procedural knowledge, which is your knowledge of the mental procedures necessary to execute tasks. If thinking is combining information in working memory, then procedural knowledge is a list of what to combine and when – it's like a recipe to accomplish a particular type of thought. You might have stored procedures for the steps needed to calculate the area of a triangle, or to duplicate a computer file using Windows, or to drive from your home to your workplace.

      There's a final necessity for thinking, which is best understood through an example. Have a look at this problem:

      In the inns of certain Himalayan villages is practiced a refined tea ceremony. The ceremony involves a host and exactly two guests, neither more nor less. When his guests have arrived and seated themselves at his table, the host performs three services for them. These services are listed in the order of the nobility the Himalayans attribute to them: stoking the fire, fanning the flames, and pouring the tea. During the ceremony, any of those present may ask another, “Honored Sir, may I perform this onerous task for you?” However, a person may request of another only the least noble of the tasks that the other is performing. Furthermore, if a person is performing any tasks, then he may not request a task that is nobler than the least noble task he is already performing. Custom requires that by the time the tea ceremony is over, all the tasks will have been transferred from the host to the most senior of the guests. How can this be accomplished?4

Schematic illustration of the tea-ceremony problem, depicted to show the analogy to the discs-and-pegs problem.