Why Don't Students Like School?. Daniel T. Willingham. Читать онлайн. Newlib. NEWLIB.NET

Автор: Daniel T. Willingham
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Серия:
Жанр произведения: Учебная литература
Год издания: 0
isbn: 9781119715801
Скачать книгу
intentionally seek out situations that demand thought.

      Solving problems brings pleasure. When I say “problem solving” in this book, I mean any cognitive work that succeeds; it might be understanding a difficult passage of prose, planning a garden, or sizing up an investment opportunity. There is a sense of satisfaction, of fulfillment, in successful thinking. Neuroscientists have found overlap between the brain areas that are important in learning and those that are important in perception of pleasure, and many neuroscientists suspect that the two systems are related. Rats in a maze learn better when rewarded with cheese. When you solve a problem or satisfy your curiosity, your brain may reward itself with a small burst of a naturally occurring chemical in the brain's pleasure system. Even though the neurochemistry is not completely understood, it seems undeniable that people take pleasure in solving problems.

      It's notable too that the pleasure is in the solving of the problem. Working on a problem with no sense that you're making progress is not pleasurable. In fact, it's frustrating. Then too, there's not great pleasure in simply knowing the answer. I told you the solution to the candle problem; did you get any fun out of it? Think how much more fun it would have been if you had solved it yourself – in fact, the problem would have seemed more clever, just as a joke that you get is funnier than a joke that has to be explained. Even if someone doesn't tell you the answer to a problem, once you've had too many hints you lose the sense that you've solved the problem, and getting the answer doesn't bring the same mental snap of satisfaction.

      The answer that most people would give may seem obvious: “I think crossword puzzles are fun and Taytay is cool, but math is boring and so is Keats.” In other words, it's the content that matters. We're curious about some stuff but not about other stuff. Certainly that's the way people describe our own interests – “I'm a stamp collector” or “I'm into medieval symphonic music.” But I don't think content drives interest. We've all attended a lecture or watched a video (perhaps only after being prevailed upon to do so) about a subject we thought we weren't interested in, only to find ourselves fascinated. And it's easy to find yourself bored even when you usually like the topic. I'll never forget my eagerness for the day my middle school teacher was to talk about sex. As a teenage boy in a staid 1970s suburban culture, I fizzed with anticipation of any talk about sex, anytime, anywhere. But when the big day came, my friends and I were bored senseless. It's not that the teacher talked about flowers and pollination – he really did talk about human sexuality – but somehow it was still dull. I actually wish I could remember how he did it; boring a bunch of hormonal teenagers with a sex talk is quite a feat.

Schematic illustration of the hard sudoku puzzle.

      Source: Sudoku © Shutterstock/Heather Wallace; geometry © Anne Carlyle Lindsay.

      So, if content is not enough to keep your attention, when does curiosity have staying power? The answer seems to lie in our judgment of how much we are likely to learn. Curiosity is maintained when we think we'll learn a lot.

Schematic illustration of a difficult-to-understand figure that will bore most people unless it is adequately introduced.

      Source: © Anne Carlyle Lindsay.

      To summarize, I've said that thinking is slow, effortful, and uncertain. Nevertheless, people like to think – or more properly, we like to think if we judge that the mental work will pay off with the pleasurable feeling we get when we learn something new. So there is no inconsistency in claiming that people avoid thought and in claiming that people are naturally curious – curiosity prompts people to explore new ideas and problems, but when we do, we quickly evaluate how much mental work it will take to solve the problem or understand what's described. If it's too much work or too little, we stop thinking about the problem if we can.

      This analysis of the sorts of mental work that people seek out or avoid also provides one answer to why more students don't like school. Working on problems that are of the right level of difficulty is rewarding, but working on problems that are too easy or too difficult is unpleasant. Students can't opt out of these problems the way adults often can. If the student routinely gets work that is a bit too difficult, it's little wonder that he doesn't care much for school. I wouldn't want to work on the Sunday New York Times crossword puzzle for several hours each day.

      So what's the solution? Give the student easier work? You could, but of course you'd have to be careful not to make it so easy that the student would be bored. And anyway, wouldn't it be better to boost the student's ability a little bit? Instead of making the work easier, is it possible to make thinking easier?

      Understanding a bit about how thinking happens will help you understand what makes thinking hard. That will in turn help you understand how to make thinking easier for your students, and therefore help them enjoy school more.

Schematic illustration of the simplest model of the mind possible.

      Source: © Greg Culley.