18
Werker, J.F., and McLeod, P.J. (1989), ‘Infant preference for both male and female infant-directed talk: A developmental study of attentional affective responsiveness’, Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43 (2), 230–246.
19
Masataka, N. (2003), The onset of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
20
Boukydis, C.F., and Burgess, R.L. (1982), ‘Adult Physiological Response to Infant Cries: Effects of Temperament of Infant, Parental Status, and Gender’, Child Development, 53 (5), 1291–1298. Swain, J.E., Lorberbaum, J.P., Kose, S., and Strathearn, L. (2007), ‘Brain basis of early parent-infant interactions: psychology, physiology, and in vivo functional neuroimaging studies’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48 (3–4), 262–287.
21
Werker, J.F., and McLeod, P.J. (1989), ‘Infant preference for both male and female infant-directed talk: A developmental study of attentional affective responsiveness’, Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43 (2), 230–246.
22
Trainor, L.J., Austin, C.M., and Desjardins, R.N. (2000), ‘Is infant directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotion?’, Psychological Science, 11 (3), 188–195.
23
Kuhl, P.K., et al. (1997), ‘Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants’, Science, 277 (5326), 684–686.
24
Burnham, D.K., Vollmer-Conna, U., and Kitamura, C. (2000), ‘Talking to infants, pets, and adults: What’s the difference?’, paper presented at the XIIth Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies, Brighton, UK.
25
Trainor, L.J., and Desjardins, R.N. (2002), ‘Pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech affect infants’ ability to discriminate vowels’, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 335–340.
26
Kitamura, C., and Lam, C. (2009), ‘Age-specific preferences for affective intent’, Infancy, 14, 77–100.
27
Sakkalou, E., and Gattis, M. (2012), ‘Infants infer intentions from prosody’, Cognitive Development, 27 (1), 1–16.
28
Pinker, S. (1994), The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. New York: HarperCollins.