Clinical Obesity in Adults and Children. Группа авторов. Читать онлайн. Newlib. NEWLIB.NET

Автор: Группа авторов
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Серия:
Жанр произведения: Медицина
Год издания: 0
isbn: 9781119695325
Скачать книгу
closely regulated [e.g. 42–44]. Campbell et al. [45] allowed 63 subjects to freely compose a diet from foods containing 10, 15, and 25% protein for 3 days (Fig. 6.4b). Subjects closely tracked a mean 14.7% protein intake, which differed highly statistically significantly from the null expectation of no selection (16.7%). It is significant that the target intakes suggested by population studies and experimental studies converge within a narrow margin of 15–17%. Interestingly, this is not dissimilar for non‐human apes studied in the wild, which range between 10% (orangutan) and 20% (mountain gorilla), with our closest living relative, the chimpanzee, selecting a diet of 13% protein [35].

      At a physiological level, there have been significant advances in understanding the mechanisms controlling macronutrient appetites [46]. Most notable has been the discovery that fibroblast growth factor (FGF)‐21 is the circulating signal of low‐protein status in humans and rodents. FGF‐21 is produced mainly in the liver and acts in the brain to stimulate protein appetite, guiding mice either to select protein‐rich foods if available or to increase intake of low‐protein diets to ensure increased protein intake, with associated increased energy intake on low‐protein, high‐energy diets [47,48]. FGF‐21 is also implicated in the inhibition of carbohydrate intake under low‐protein, high‐carbohydrate feeding in mice and humans [49–52].

      Response to variation in dietary macronutrient balance: protein leverage

      Schematic illustration of dietary macronutrient regulation in humans. Schematic illustration of dietary macronutrient regulation in humans.

      Source: Adapted from Campbell et al. [45].

      That total energy intake is indeed leveraged by protein in humans has now been demonstrated in several controlled experimental studies [45,56–60] and in secondary analysis of compiled literature data [61,62] (Fig. 6.5c). The studies of Gosby et al. [57], conducted in Sydney, and Campbell et al. [45], in Jamaica, disguised the macronutrient composition of experimental foods and controlled for palatability, variety, and availability differences between treatments. Subjects were provided with menus containing 10, 15, or 25% protein for 4 or 5 days. Some foods and snacks were savory and others sweet in flavor characteristics, but all were of the same macronutrient composition for a given experimental period. Although the nature of the experimental foods and menus differed between Sydney and Jamaica for cultural reasons [63], the outcomes in terms of nutrient and energy intakes were closely similar between the two studies, with subjects ingesting most calories on the 10% protein diet. Notably, in the Sydney study, subjects ingested 12% more calories on the 10% protein diet than on the 15% protein diet, with these excess calories coming mainly from increased snacking between meals on savory‐flavored food options [57]. This behavior was associated with elevated FGF‐21 levels on the 10% protein diet [48]. Hence, subjects on the 10% protein treatment diet demonstrated behavioral and physiological characteristics of protein‐seeking behavior. The increased salience of savory (umami) flavor cues when in a state of protein deficit has also been shown in brain imaging studies [64,65].

      Schematic illustration of response of human appetite systems to variation in dietary macronutrient ratios. Schematic illustration of response of human appetite systems to variation in dietary macronutrient ratios. Schematic illustration of response of human appetite systems to variation in dietary macronutrient ratios.