Fig. 97.—Experiment for sight.
This faculty of accommodation in the eye is thus demonstrated: we place two pins, one in front of the other, one eye only being open; we first look at the nearest pin, which appears confused if it is near the eye, but by an effort of will the image becomes clear. If, while preserving the clearness of the image, we then carry our attention to the second pin, we find that it, too, presents a confused appearance. If we make an effort to distinguish the contour of the second pin, we at last succeed, and the first once more appears ill-defined. It is only since the experiments of M. Cramer and M. Helmholtz that the explanation of this phenomenon could have been given. M. Cramer has succeeded in determining on the living eye the curved ray of the cornea, and of the two surfaces of the crystalline lens. In so doing he followed Samson’s method, and observed the images thrown by a luminous object, whose rays strike the different refracting surfaces of the eye. A candle, L (fig. 98), is placed before the eye, O, and throws as in a convex mirror a straight image of the flame, A (fig. 99). The other portion of the light, which has penetrated the pupil, falls on the crystalline lens, and produces likewise a second straight image, B. Then the light refracted by the lens reaches the posterior surface; a portion is reflected on a concave mirror, and gives the inverted image, C, very small and brilliant. M. Cramer observed it through a microscope, and studied the variations in the size of images when the eye passed from the observation of adjacent to distant objects. He stated:—
Fig. 98.—M. Cramer’s experiment.
Fig. 99.—Images in the eye.
1. That the image, A, formed on the surface of the cornea, remains the same size in both cases; the form of the cornea therefore remains unaltered.
2. That the image, B, formed on the upper surface of the lens, diminishes in proportion as the eye is nearer the object; the surface therefore becoming more and more convex, as the focal distance diminishes—a result indicated by the theory that it is possible in the vision of near objects to receive the image on the retina.
3. That the third image, C, produced on the posterior surface of the lens, remains nearly invariable.
We may confirm Cramer’s statements by an easy experiment. We place ourselves in front of the eye of someone who looks in turn at two objects placed on the same black line at unequal distances from him, and are able to distinguish by the dimension of the images of the candle, which object it is that he is regarding. M. Helmholtz has carried M. Cramer’s methods to perfection, and has been able to formulate a complete theory of all the phenomena of accommodation. The laws of optics show that the rays emitted by a luminous point may unite at another point by the action of the refracting surfaces of the eye. Nevertheless, a white light being composed of rays of diverse refrangibility, particular effects, known under the name of chromatic aberration, are produced through the decomposition of light, which we will proceed to study, under M. Helmholtz’s auspices11. We make a narrow opening in a screen, and fix behind this opening a violet glass, penetrable only by red and violet rays. We then place a light, the red rays of which reach the eye of the observer after having passed through the glass and the opening in the screen. If the eye is adapted to the red rays, the violet rays will form a circle of diffusion, and a red point encircled with a violet aureola is seen. The eye may also be brought to a state of refraction, so that the point of convergence of the violet rays is in front, and that of the red rays behind the retina, the diameters of the red and violet circles of diffusion being equal. It is then only that the luminous point appears monochromatic. When the eye is in this state of refraction, the simple rays, whose refrangibility is maintained between the red and the violet rays, unite on the retina.
There is another kind of aberration of luminous rays of one colour emitted through a hole, which generally only approach approximately to a mathematical focus, in consequence of the properties of refracting surfaces; it is called aberration of sphericity. The phenomena are as follows:—
Fig. 100.
1. We take for our object a very small luminous point (the hole made by a pin in some black paper, through which the light passes), and having also placed before the eye a convex glass, if we are not near-sighted, we fix it a little beyond the point of accommodation, so that it produces on the retina a little circle of diffusion. We then see, instead of the luminous point, a figure representing from four to eight irregular rays, which generally differ with both eyes, and also with different people. We have given the result of M. Helmholtz’s observations in fig. 100; a corresponds to the right eye, and b to the left. The outer edges of the luminous parts of an image, produced in this way by a white light, are bordered with blue; the edges towards the centre are of a reddish yellow. The writer adds that the figure appears to him to have greater length than breadth. If the light is feeble, only the most brilliant parts of the figure can be seen, and several images of the luminous point are visible, of which one is generally more brilliant than the others. If, on the other hand, the light is very intense—if, for example, the direct light of the sun passes through a small opening—the rays mingle with each other, and are surrounded by aureola of rays, composed of numberless extremely fine lines, of all colours, possessing a much larger diameter, and which we distinguish by the name of the aureola of capillary rays.
Fig. 101.
The radiating form of stars, and the distant light of street-lamps belong to the preceding phenomena. If the eye is accommodated to a greater distance than that of the luminous point—and for this purpose, if the luminous point itself is distant, we place before the eye a slightly convex lens—we see another radiating image appear, which M. Helmholtz represents thus (fig. 101): at c as it is presented to the right eye, and at d as seen by the left.
If the pupil is covered on one side, the side opposite to the image of diffusion disappears; that is to say, that part of the retinal image situated on the same side as the covered half of the pupil. This figure, then, is formed by rays which have not yet crossed the axis of the eye. If we place the luminous point at a distance to which the eye can accommodate itself, we see, through a moderate light, a small, round, luminous spot, without any irregularities. If the light, on the contrary, is intense, the image is radiated in every position of accommodation, and we merely find that on approaching nearer, the figure which was elongated, answering to a distant accommodation, gradually diminishes, grows rounder, and gives place to the vertically elongated figure, which belongs to the accommodation of a nearer point. When we examine a slender, luminous line, we behold images developed, which are easily foreseen, if for every point of the line we suppose radiating images of diffusion, which encroach on each other. The clearest portions of these images of diffusion mingle together and form distinct lines, which show multiplied images of the luminous line. Most persons will see two of these images; some, with the eyes in certain positions, will see five or six.
Fig.