The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. Группа авторов. Читать онлайн. Newlib. NEWLIB.NET

Автор: Группа авторов
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Серия:
Жанр произведения: Общая психология
Год издания: 0
isbn: 9781119678991
Скачать книгу
findings suggested that everyday parent–child discussions encompassing moral themes (e.g., rules, issues, conflicts) promoted growth in children’s moral reasoning (Dunn, 2006) and that positive parent–child relations fostered growth in children’s conscience and moral behavior (Kochanska et al., 2010). Investigation of the relative merits of inductive as opposed to assertive or hostile discipline revealed that, whereas induction forecasted children’s prosocial‐moral beliefs and behavior (Hart et al. 2003), assertive and hostile discipline predicted antithetical outcomes (Baumrind et al., 2010).

      Cultural contrasts. Researchers also compared Western socialization practices to those found in other cultures. In cross‐national comparisons, for example, differences were found in parent’s perceptions of their children’s temperamental and behavioral characteristics (Russell et al., 2003), but similarities were reported for the consequences of particular parenting styles (e.g., authoritarian parenting and child aggression; Nelson et al., 2014). Other findings showed that parenting effects varied depending on the family’s ethnicity and cultural context. To illustrate, researchers found that, when compared to White or Hispanic youth, African‐American adolescents were less likely to participate in gangs or gang‐related delinquency if their parents abstained from lax discipline and wielded greater control over children’s behavior (Walker‐Barnes & Mason, 2001).

      The peer context

      After a period of dormancy following World War II, research on children’s peer relations reemerged in the 1970s and expanded thereafter (Ladd, 2005). A key impetus was the premise that peer socialization prepared children to succeed in multiple spheres of life, including romantic and workplace relations.

      Evidence linking children’s behavior with the quality of their peer relationships led investigators to search for the origins of social “competencies” and skill “deficits” (Ladd, 2005). Among the determinants examined were the social‐cognitive underpinnings (Gifford‐Smith & Rabiner, 2004) and the parenting and family processes associated with children’s behavior amongst peers (Ladd & Kochenderfer‐Ladd, 2019).

      Another key objective was to confirm and extend early longitudinal findings suggesting that the quality of children’s peer relations during childhood predicted their health and adjustment in adolescence and adulthood. A new wave of prospective longitudinal studies largely substantiated this premise. Childhood peer rejection, victimization, and friendlessness forecasted a variety of later‐life social difficulties and dysfunctions (Ladd, 2005).

      Peer relations research eventually broadened to incorporate diverse ethnic and cultural contexts. Within North America, for example, researchers discovered that whereas Euro‐American children had more cross‐ethnic friendships than African‐American children (Kawabata & Crick, 2008), African‐American children had a larger number of friendships and more opposite‐sex friendships (Kovacs et al., 1996). Internationally, research on bullying that had originated in Norway spread to many other nations. Additionally, cross‐national comparisons were made of children’s friendships, peer group relations, social behavior, and interpersonal competencies (Chen et al. 2018).

      Childcare and schooling contexts

      Cultural and economic changes made childcare and formal schooling foci for social development research. A primary aim for childcare research, as detailed later, was to elucidate the impact of early nonparental care on young children’s socioemotional development. Another investigative thrust centered on the evaluation of compensatory programs for economically disadvantaged preschoolers. Programs such as Head Start, which were designed to prepare children for school, eventually expanded their objectives to include social as well as pre‐academic competencies (Raver & Zigler, 1997).

      Research on after‐school arrangements for school‐age children arose in response to the growth of dual‐earner families. Studies of self‐care (i.e., allowing children to look after themselves after school) often documented hazards and risks (e.g., stress, drug use, antisocial behavior; Lord & Mahoney, 2007). In contrast, children were found to benefit from structured, adult‐supervised after‐school programs (Vandell et al., 2005).

      Media

      Media, in all of its rapidly expanding forms, received substantial investigative attention. Early research focused on televised violence and its effects on children’s aggressive behavior. Corroboration of this effect and the pervasiveness of violence in media aimed at children (Wilson et al., 2002) spurred additional lines of investigation. Included were studies designed to explicate violent television’s role in fostering hostile attitudes and aggressive, violent, and delinquent behavior (Bushman & Huesmann, 2012). Researchers also examined media’s effects on children’s social relations, perceptions, and emotional sensitivity. Findings indicated that media use not only limited children’s participation in real‐life peer relations (e.g., friendships) and social activities (Pea et al., 2012), but it also distorted their perceptions of the social world. Illustrations included results showing that children, after viewing episodes of interpersonal conflict, developed negative expectations toward unknown peers (i.e., perceived hypothetical classmates as unfriendly; Mares et al., 2012). Additionally, evidence suggested (although see Ferguson, 2007) that violent media and video games desensitized children to violence, reduced their emotional responsiveness, and fostered stereotypes (Bushman & Huesmann, 2012).

      It also became evident that media need not be harmful and in fact, could facilitate children’s social development. Research on educational and public‐service television revealed that, depending on its form and content, media could not only discourage antisocial behavior but also increase altruism. Investigators discovered, for example, that children who watched Sesame Street and Mister Rogers – TV programs rich in sociomoral and prosocial content – were more likely to learn and apply prosocial behaviors in real‐life interactions (Mares & Woodward, 2001).

      The introduction of the internet (i.e., social media) and its rapid adoption by youth prompted research on its use and impact (Livingstone & Haddon, 2009). Both benefits and risks were identified. For example, whereas evidence showed that children utilized these platforms to meet and maintain friendships (Wolak et al., 2002), it also revealed that internet usage made them vulnerable to cyberbullying and abuse by predators (Ybarra et al., 2006).

       Aim 2: Delineate the biological foundations, mechanisms, and processes that launch, regulate, and shape the course of social development

      Theory and research on the biological foundations of human