69 Rapee, R.M., Kim, J., Wang, J., Liu, X., Hofmann, S.G., Chen, J., Oh, K.Y., Bögels, S.M., Arman, S., Heinrichs, N., & Alden, L. E. (2011). Perceived impact of socially anxious behaviors on individuals' lives in Western and East Asian countries. Behavior Therapy, 42, 485–492.
70 Ren, D., Wesselmann, E., & Williams, K.D. (2016). Evidence for another response to ostracism: solitude seeking. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(3), 204–212.
71 Rideout, V. & Robb, M.B. (2018). Social Media, Social Life: Teens Reveal Their Experiences. Common Sense Media. San Francisco: CA: Common Sense Media.
72 Rubin, K.H. (1982). Non‐social play in preschoolers: necessary evil? Child Development, 53, 651–657.
73 Rubin, K.H., Bukowski, W.M., & Bowker, J.C. (2015). Children in peer groups. In R.M. Lerner (Series Ed.), M.H. Bornstein, & T. Leventhal (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, Vol. 4: Ecological Settings and Processes in Developmental Systems (7th ed., pp. 175–222). New York: Wiley‐Blackwell.
74 Rubin, K.H., Coplan, R.J., & Bowker, J.C. (2009). Social withdrawal in childhood. The Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 141–171.
75 Rubin, K.H. & Mills, R.S. (1988). The many faces of social isolation in childhood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 916–924.
76 Scott, G.G., Boyle, E.A., Czerniawska, K., & Courtney, A. (2018). Posting photos on Facebook: the impact of narcissism, social anxiety, loneliness, and shyness. Personality and Individual Differences, 133, 67–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.12.039
77 Shipley, T.E. & Veroff, J.A. (1952). A projective measure of need for affiliation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, 349–356.
78 Schreier, S.S., Heinrichs, N., Alden, L., Rapee, R.M., Hofmann, S.G., Chen, J., & Bögels, S. (2010). Social anxiety and social norms in individualistic and collectivistic countries. Depression and Anxiety, 27, 1128–1134.
79 Staats, H. & Hartig, T. (2004). Alone or with a friend: a social context for psychological restoration and environmental preferences. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 199–211.
80 Storr, A. (1988). Solitude: A Return to the Self. New York: Free Press.
81 Sullivan, H.S. (1953). The Interpersonal Theory of Psychiatry. New York: W.W. Norton & Co.
82 Thomas, A. & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and Development. Brunner/Mazel.
83 Thoreau, H.D. (1854). Walden; or, Life in the Woods. Boston: Ticknor & Fields.
84 Trivers, R. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.
85 Valtorta, N.K., Kanaan, M., Gilbody, S., & Hanratty, B. (2018). Loneliness, social isolation and risk of cardiovascular disease in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. European Journal of Preventive Cardiology, 25, 1387–1396.
86 Van Roekel, E., Scholte, R., Engels, R., Goossens, L., & Verhagen, M. (2015). Loneliness in the daily lives of adolescents: an experience sampling study examining the effects of social contexts. Journal of Early Adolescence, 35, 905–930.
87 Williams, K.D. & Nida, S.A. (2011). Ostracism: consequences and coping. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 71–75.
88 Wilson, T.D., Reinhard, D.A., Westgate, E.C., Gilbert, D.T., Ellerbeck, N., Hahn, C., … & Shaked, A. (2014). Just think: the challenges of the disengaged mind. Science, 345, 75–77.
89 Winnicott, D.W. (1958). The capacity to be alone. In: The Maturational Processes and the Facilitating Environment (pp. 29–36, 1979). New York: International Universities Press.
90 Xu, Y., Farver, J.A.M., Chang, L., Zhang, Z., & Yu, L. (2007). Moving away or fitting in? Understanding shyness in Chinese children. Merrill‐Palmer Quarterly, 53, 527–556.
91 Zimmerman, J.G. (1805). Advantages of solitude upon the mind. (pp. 119–229). London, UK: James Cundee, London.
2 Evolutionary and Neuroscientific Perspectives on Adaptive Shyness
Raha Hassan*, Taigan L. MacGowan, Kristie L. Poole, and Louis A. Schmidt
McMaster University, Canada
Shyness reflects inhibition and anxiousness in social situations, and has been conceptualized as an emotion as well as an enduring characteristic of one’s personality (see Jones et al., 1986). Although shyness is a ubiquitous phenomenon that is observed across development, with estimates exceeding 90% of individuals experiencing it at some points in their lives (Zimbardo, 1977), a smaller percentage of children (~10–15%; Kagan, 1994) and adults (< 40%; Pilkonis, 1977) are characterized as dispositionally or temperamentally shy. Temperamental shyness is associated with a number of distinct physiological correlates of stress‐vulnerability, including greater relative right frontal EEG activity, high and stable heart rate at rest, and high morning basal cortisol levels (see Kagan et al., 1988; Schmidt & Miskovic, 2014; Schmidt & Schulkin, 1999, for reviews). Interestingly, these same distinct patterns of resting physiology have been identified in other mammals, including timid and cautious nonhuman primates (see Shackman et al., 2013), suggesting that temperamental shyness may be conserved across mammals. Temperamental shyness also has been linked to a range of internalizing problems (Findlay et al., 2009), but primarily social anxiety (Heiser et al., 2003; Hofmann et al., 2006; Poole et al., 2017). However, we know that not all individuals who are shy experience maladjustment. Some individuals who are shy appear to adapt reasonably well (Schmidt et al., 2017; Tang et al., 2017).
Shyness is an inherently interesting phenomenon to study, not only because of its ubiquity, but because social interaction and social connection are so fundamental to human existence, raising questions regarding the function of shyness and what purpose(s) it serves (see Schmidt & Poole, 2020a). In this chapter, we explore this broader question from evolutionary and neuroscientific perspectives. To this end, we address three specific questions organized around the broader former question: (1) Are there adaptive functions of shyness? (2) What are some of the regulatory mechanisms of adaptive shyness? and (3) How are these self‐regulatory mechanisms instantiated in the brain in adaptive shyness?
Are There Adaptive Functions of Shyness?
Temperaments are early‐emerging, biologically based, and stable traits that can provide individuals with diverse behavioral strategies that allow them to gain access to resources, reproduce, and coexist within a social hierarchy (Kagan, 1994). The shy‐bold continuum, for example, is commonly observed in nonhuman animal species of fish, birds, and mammals (Wilson et al., 1994). Within this continuum, some individuals are more biologically inclined to exhibit risk‐taking behavior and approach toward novel stimuli (i.e., bold behavioral strategy) whereas others will display fear and avoidance in response to unfamiliar objects, individuals, and situations (i.e., shy behavioral strategy; Groothuis & Carere, 2005; Koolhaas et al., 1999; Wilson et al., 1994). Similarly, temperamental behavioral inhibition can be assessed in humans, with these observed tendencies being evident from infancy throughout development (Kagan, 1994). While behavioral inhibition and the shy‐bold continuum tend to focus on the extent to which an individual experiences approach or avoidance motivations toward any unfamiliar stimulus, there are also individual differences in responses to unfamiliar stimuli that are of a social nature. Specifically, some individuals will exhibit bolder behaviors with social conspecifics while others tend to experience fear and anxiety when interacting with unfamiliar social partners and when encountering new social situations (see Schmidt & Schulkin, 1999, for a review). Although both responses can be viewed as adaptive, wariness and fear are not always acknowledged to have value in our evolutionary past or in more recent human history. Given how conserved the shy‐bold continuum and phenotype appear to be across a range of animal