Animal Behavior for Shelter Veterinarians and Staff. Группа авторов. Читать онлайн. Newlib. NEWLIB.NET

Автор: Группа авторов
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Серия:
Жанр произведения: Биология
Год издания: 0
isbn: 9781119618508
Скачать книгу
(Skinner 1956). Skinner was running an experiment in which a rat pressed a lever for food. Unbeknownst to Skinner, the pellet dispenser jammed at some point during the session. Therefore, presses on the lever no longer produced reinforcement and underwent extinction. The rat didn’t immediately stop pressing the lever; instead, there was a gradual reduction in the number of lever presses before the behavior finally stopped. Skinner’s accidental demonstration of extinction highlights an important feature of the process: the behavior under extinction diminishes gradually, not in an all‐or‐none fashion. How quickly behavior decreases during extinction is a function of the schedule of reinforcement that maintained the behavior, the length of time the behavior has been in the animal’s repertoire, and whether conditioned reinforcers are delivered during extinction.

      Extinction and reinforcement are used in combination to teach new behaviors through a technique called shaping. In shaping, a behavior is trained by reinforcing responses with forms that are closer and closer to a final desired behavior. In the laboratory, a common scenario is for a rat to press a lever for food. However, when a rat is put in the operant chamber for the very first time, it is highly unlikely that he would press the lever since the lever‐press response and the reinforcer have yet to be associated. Experimenters must first shape the lever‐press response before they can run their experiments. As the rat sniffs around the operant chamber and looks in the direction of the lever, the experimenter delivers a food pellet. As the rat moves progressively closer, each approach is reinforced with a food pellet. The experimenter might then wait for the rat to place his paw on the lever before delivering food. And finally, the rat presses down on the lever, exhibiting the final desired behavior. As successive approximations to a lever press are reinforced, previous responses that had formerly been reinforced are extinguished.

      3.4.3 Stimulus Control

Description Example
Antecedent A stimulus that precedes a response Mailperson walks down the street
Behavior The organism’s response to the antecedent The dog barks
Consequence The stimulus change that follows the behavior (addition or removal of a stimulus) The mailperson crosses the street and thereby reinforces the dog’s barking behavior

      In technical terms, if the presentation of a stimulus reliably evokes an operant response, the stimulus is called a “discriminative stimulus.” In application, a discriminative stimulus is often called a “cue.” For a stimulus to reliably function as a discriminative stimulus, the same rules for creating strong associations apply. The cue needs to reliably and consistently signal a certain consequence if a behavior occurs. Naive trainers sometimes attempt to train their pet to sit by repeatedly saying “sit.” After saying “sit” a dozen times, the pet sits and gets a treat. Unfortunately, “sit” never becomes a reliable cue because the pet did not sit most of the time the cue was presented. However, after a few pairings of the trainer saying “sit” once and the dog sits, and the dog is unlikely to sit when the trainer refrains from saying “sit,” then the behavior can be said to be under stimulus control.

      Antecedent stimuli can reliably evoke behavior after being correlated with reinforcement. Antecedent stimuli can also reliably inhibit behavior by signaling that there is no chance that reinforcement will occur following a behavior (i.e., extinction). A stimulus that signals extinction is termed s‐delta. Stimuli that signal whether a behavior will be reinforced or not are ubiquitous and very effective in guiding behavior. An “Out of Order” sign on a vending machine tells us that using the machine will not yield us any goodies. In animal behavior, research has found that dogs don’t beg from people who aren’t looking at them (Udell et al. 2011). A person not looking at the dog is effectively an “Out of Order” sign to the dog that means if the dog begs, it is highly unlikely that she will get food. If the person is making eye contact with the dog, it is more likely that food will be given.

      One area of training in which “tight” stimulus control is desired is landmine detection. A non‐profit organization called Anti‐Persoonsmijnen Ontmijnende Product Ontwikkeling (APOPO) employed the use of giant pouched rats to help with de‐mining areas of Africa (Poling et al. 2010). The rats are given extensive training on detecting the odor of a landmine and emitting a behavior to indicate that they found an explosive. So that each landmine is identified and removed safely, it is imperative that the explosives have perfect stimulus control over the indicator behavior. That is, the behavior must occur each time a landmine is found, it must never occur when there is no landmine present, it must never occur in response to a different stimulus, and no other behavior should occur in the presence of the landmine (Pryor 1999). Otherwise, it would be a waste of resources to dig for a landmine that isn’t there and would put lives at risk if a landmine is missed.

      3.4.3.1 Discrimination and Generalization

      After some experience learning a target behavior with one person, a dog might respond to a discriminative stimulus (such as “sit”) from other people. Responding to the same cue from a different person is an instance of generalization. Conversely, if the dog does not