Doing Field Projects. John Forrest. Читать онлайн. Newlib. NEWLIB.NET

Автор: John Forrest
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Серия:
Жанр произведения: Культурология
Год издания: 0
isbn: 9781119734628
Скачать книгу
Like anthropologists of the nineteenth century, their data were second-hand. They did no fieldwork for themselves. As social science matured in the early twentieth century the split between sociology and anthropology opened up, with anthropologists carving out intensive, long-term fieldwork projects with peoples well outside of Europe and North America as their domain – in large part, supported by, and promoted by, colonialist governments for their own ends. In this context, getting out of their armchairs and getting to grips with the pragmatics of rigorous, “scientific” (that is, grounded and verifiable) fieldwork in foreign places was a major breakthrough in the development of anthropology as a discipline.

      The Evolution Century

      In many ways we can think of the nineteenth century as the “evolution century.” The notion that things evolve according to scientifically discoverable principles became the mantra across the board in academia. Wilhelm and Jacob Grimm, famous for their folktale collections, were significant linguists who documented in detail how languages evolved, Charles Lyell suggested that rocks and the physical landscape evolved according to discoverable scientific principles, Mary Anning found fossil evidence of change in lifeforms over time, and Charles Darwin (as well as Alfred Russel Wallace) argued that species evolve via the process of natural selection. Evolution seemed like the perfect paradigm to cover all branches of inquiry, and the study of human cultures looked as if it could easily fit under the umbrella of evolution along with the others. E. B. Tylor in Britain (Tylor 1871) and Lewis Henry Morgan (Morgan 1877) in North America developed complementary paradigms of universal sociocultural evolution that became normative in anthropology until the early twentieth century. Both Tylor and Morgan argued that the movement through various stages – which Morgan labeled savagery, barbarism, and civilization – was controlled by discoverable scientific principles akin to the three-age system which is still in use (in considerably modified form) in many branches of Old World archeology.

      Archeologists now see that the three-age system is neither applicable to all cultures worldwide nor as logical a series of steps as once appeared obvious. Some cultures never developed metallurgy indigenously, yet they managed to evolve in complex ways sociopolitically. Some cultures had a distinct copper-using era (the Chalcolithic) in between their Stone and Bronze Ages. Some cultures skipped over the Bronze Age entirely and moved directly from stone tools to iron ones. The possibilities are seemingly endless, and what was once presumed to be a series of inevitable advancements taken in logical steps is now seen to be a much more fluid and malleable process, influenced by a variety of variables including geography and contact with other cultures. In the nineteenth century, however, such nuances were less well known because both archeological methods and ethnographic fieldwork were in their infancy. Reliable data were scarce.

      While the 1870s were productive in North America in some spheres of ethnographic fieldwork, such as Morgan’s extensive collection of kinship data, first from the Iroquois (Morgan 1851), and later from the Winnebago, Crow, Yankton, Kaw, Blackfeet, Omaha, and others (Morgan 1871), there were two significant problems with the research. First, Morgan relied almost exclusively on interviews, which meant that the information which he collected had no cultural context with which to frame it and, hence, make sense of it, and, second, because there was a physical and intellectual separation between museum departments, which were engaged in field expeditions, and university teaching departments of anthropology, which might have been bastions of ethnographic theory but were virtually nonexistent. Consequently, field data did not translate into theory within a university environment. Morgan never held an academic position but, almost by default (there being no rivals in academia), his paradigm of universal cultural evolution held center stage, and was heavily relied upon by all manner of theorists, from Karl Marx to Sigmund Freud, to buttress their own theories of social development.

      The nineteenth century also saw the pinnacle of European colonialism across the globe. This colonialism was fueled by greed, and supported by racist assumptions that colonized peoples were inferior to their colonial masters and, therefore, could be exploited with impunity. To be profitable, industries in Europe, especially Britain, needed cheap raw materials and cheap labor, which colonialism provided in abundance. Slavery was an outgrowth of the colonial system, justified by a stance that non-European peoples were inferior, racially and culturally to Europeans. Civilizing them was the “White man’s burden.” The anthropology that developed in Europe in the early twentieth century was profoundly shaped by this colonialist mentality in complex ways, especially within the imperial holdings of European nations.

      Off the Verandah and into the Colonies

      At the beginning of the twentieth century, British anthropologists started to emulate their colleagues in North America by journeying well away from home to conduct fieldwork. The turning point for British anthropology was the Torres Straits expedition of 1898 led by psychologist-turned-anthropologist, W.H.R. Rivers, and including C.G. Seligman, who later taught Bronislaw Malinowski, E.E. Evans-Pritchard, and Meyer Fortes. The Torres Straits lie between the northern tip of Queensland in Australia and the southeastern shores of Papua New Guinea, and their indigenous island peoples are culturally and linguistically distinct from either, and from each other.

      The six members of the expedition were an assortment of psychologists, linguists, and ethnologists, and their preferred method of fieldwork was interviewing as well as taking physical measurements. In the process, Rivers discovered that the people he interviewed had the same visual abilities as Europeans, yet had no word for the color blue, and used the same word for blue things as for black things. Likewise, they did not have words to distinguish biological siblings and cousins. Inspired by his work in the Torres Straits, Rivers spent several months in 1901–2 among the Todas of the Nilgiri Hills of southern India.