Cognitive Flexibility. Evelyne Clement. Читать онлайн. Newlib. NEWLIB.NET

Автор: Evelyne Clement
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Серия:
Жанр произведения: Учебная литература
Год издания: 0
isbn: 9781119902720
Скачать книгу

      Whether in practice or in research, we may wish to use more playful situations to assess flexibility, depending on the age or specificity of the participants. The following two situations, for example, can be used for this purpose.

      The innovation paradigm consists of asking the child to draw an object that does not exist (Karmiloff-Smith 1990). The child must first draw objects that really exist (house, man, animal) and then draw these same objects, but also those that do not exist in our world (Picard and Vinter 2005). Young children (4 to 6 years) make intracategorical changes, that is, they only change the size or shape of the object itself, without introducing intercategorical changes. These changes appear later (8 to 10 years) and correspond to the introduction of an element of one category (the wings of a bird for example) on a drawing of another category (a house).

      Another fun way to assess flexibility in young children, for example, is to use a drawing of a character starting with the foot (Baldy 2010) to assess the possibility of switching to another procedure in a flexible way. This involves slowing down the routine of drawing a man, which usually always starts with the head, by requiring the child to start the drawing with one of the man’s feet.

Authors Population Type of measurement
Trail Making Test Reitan and Wolfson (1993) Adults Reactive Item by item
Trail Making Test for preschoolers Espy and Cwik (2004) Preschoolers Reactive Item by item
Color Trail Test D’Elia et al. (1996) Adults Reactive Item by item
Children’s Color Trail Test Williams et al. (1995) Children aged 8 to 16 years old Reactive Item by item
Plus minus Jersild (1927) Adults Reactive Task switching
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Grant and Berg (1948) Adults Reactive Per block
Brixton Spatial Anticipation Test Burgess and Shallice (1996) Adults Reactive Per block
Brixton Preschool Lehto and Uusitalo (2006) Preschoolers Reactive Per block
Dimensional Change Card Sort Doebel and Zelazo (2015) Preschoolers Reactive Per block
Local Global Task Miyake et al. (2000) Adults Reactive Task switching
Number-letter task Rogers and Monsell (1995) Adults Reactive Task switching
The color-shape task Miyake and Friedman (2012) Adults Reactive Task switching
Verbal fluency Delis et al. (2001) 8–89 years old (norm) Spontaneous
The Creature Counting Manly et al. (2001) Children aged 6 to 12 years old (norm) Reactive Per block
Fluidity of drawings Korkman et al. (2012) Children aged 5 to 16 years and 11 months (norm) Spontaneous
Categorization Korkman et al. (2012) Children aged 7 to 16 years and 11 months (norm) Spontaneous
Innovation paradigm Karmiloff-Smith (1990) Children Spontaneous
Drawing of a man starting with the foot Baldy (2010) Children Spontaneous

      1.3.1.2. Indirect assessments

      Several questionnaires allow for the indirect assessment of cognitive flexibility. One of the first to be proposed was the Cognitive Flexibility Scale (Martin and Ruben 1995). This scale consists of 12 items, to which the participant responds using a five-point Likert scale. Example of an item: “I am willing to work on problems that require a creative solution”.

      The next three correspond to three versions of the Behavioral Assessment of Executive Functions Inventory, depending on the population. These three scales were constructed on an ecological basis and propose to evaluate the consequences of executive deficits in daily life, by adapting the situations to the three age groups targeted (preschoolers, children/adolescents, adults).

      The BRIEF-A (Behavioral Assessment of Executive Function