The executive who does not realize the value from analytics or fails to adopt will be replaced by an executive who can deliver insights for data-driven decisions. This is inevitable because executives who fail to do so will endanger their company’s performance and competitive position.
HUMAN JUDGMENT AND DECISION-MAKING
In business, human decision-making does not always optimize performance because it is vulnerable to bias and intuition: that is, gut feel. We are naturally intuitive about the future but quantitatively limited to calculate what the future probably can be. We react to events and rely on experience to “guide” us to a decision. We also may have a personal want or need that influences and impacts our decisions.
As such, we must first understand how nature has wired us to make decisions before we can appreciate and accept how analytics can contribute to enhancing decision-making that can lead to improved business performance. The need to balance our instinctive judgment with AI for decisions is necessary to fulfill the potential value of analytics in business and avoid the shortcomings associated with traditional decision-making.
The research of Kahneman and Tversky, who received the Nobel Prize for Economics in 2002, produced a ground-breaking understanding of human judgment and decision-making under uncertainty. Their research is viewed as one of the most influential social science behavioral insights of the past century. It challenged the notion held by many economists that the human mind is unconsciously rational.
Kahneman authored a book, Thinking, Fast and Slow; the central thesis is the interplay between what he terms System 1 and System 2 thinking.3 In System 1, a person has an instinctual response that is automatic and rapid and has been shaped by experience and expertise. For example, how much is 2 plus 2? Hopefully, you said 4. Your response was immediate and almost instinctive because, over many years, this simple answer has always been the same. In effect, System 1 seeks coherence and applies relevant memories to explain events or make decisions.
System 2 is invoked for more complex, thoughtful reasoning and is characterized by slower, more rational analysis but is “prone to laziness and fatigue.” If you want to conduct your own experiment along these lines, ask someone to write down the results of a hypothetical sequence of 20 coin flips. Then ask the person to flip a coin 20 times and write down the results. The actual flips will almost certainly contain streaks of only heads or tails—the sorts of streaks that people do not think a random coin produces on its own. This kind of misconception leads us to incorrectly analyze all sorts of situations in business, politics, and everyday life.
Further, the research of Kahneman and Tversky revealed previously undiscovered patterns of human irrationality: the ways that our minds consistently fool us and the steps we can take, at least some of the time, to avoid being fooled. They used the word heuristics to describe the rules of thumb that often lead people astray.
One such rule is the halo effect, in which thinking about one positive attribute of a person or thing causes observers to perceive other strengths that are not actually there. For example, a project team was discussing the status of a new marketing campaign. The campaign was led by Billy, who had a reputation for delivering successful campaigns. Team members were asked to give their assessment of progress and, recognizing Billy’s past successes, gave positive evaluations. This reflected the halo effect in that the past successes extended to this project without any factual basis other than Billy’s reputation.
This work has led to advances in individual behavior. It is full of practical little ideas like “No one ever made a decision because of a number”; Kahneman has said, “They need a story.” Or Tversky’s theory of socializing: because stinginess and generosity are both contagious, and because behaving generously makes you happier, surround yourself with generous people.
The research has clarified how decisions are made and underlying influences that can impact decisions. These influences are inherent in group interactions and individual biases, which are key to understanding the balance between human judgment and analytics in decision-making.
Group Decision-Making
Several recognized behavioral group decision-making processes occur in forms that are considered flawed because they contain bias. They lack the tools of analytics to inject unbiased insights into the decision process. One of these occurrences is often referred to as the Abilene paradox, where a group of people collectively decide on a course of action that is counter to the preferences of many or all of the individuals in the group.4 It involves a common breakdown of group communication in which each member mistakenly believes that their own preferences are counter to the group’s and, therefore, does not raise objections. A common phrase relating to the Abilene paradox is a desire to “not rock the boat.”
For example, the design team of a successful smartphone is deciding whether to remove the home button on its next version release. The lead designer suggests that the home button be kept, and the decision, after some discussion, is to keep the home button. Later that day, some of the design team meet for lunch, and Peter expresses his preference for removing the home button. Mickey jumps in to say “Me too!” and is followed by Davey. They all acquiesced to the decision since they believed they were the only ones who did not agree. In fact, when the team reassembled, most of the other members also preferred to remove the home button but also did not express their preference.
Another group decision-making process is groupthink, a mode of thinking in which individual members of small cohesive groups tend to accept a decision that represents a perceived group consensus, whether or not the group members believe it to be valid, correct, or optimal.5 Groupthink reduces the efficiency of collective problem solving within such groups and perpetuates bias and flawed assumptions.
For example, a capital project review team is convened to decide on next year’s CapEx budget. Each member is asked to indicate their preferred #1 project. After the first and second members express their preference for the same project, each succeeding member agrees with the first two, although their individual preferences were different. They “go along to get along” and express their agreement with the preferred project.
Ronald Sims writes that the Abilene paradox is like groupthink but differs in significant ways, including that in groupthink, individuals are not acting contrary to their conscious wishes and generally feel good about the decisions the group has reached.6 According to Sims, in the Abilene paradox, the individuals acting contrary to their own wishes are more likely to have negative feelings about the outcome. In Sims’ view, groupthink is a psychological phenomenon affecting clarity of thought, whereas in the Abilene paradox, thought is unaffected.
These group decision-making processes demonstrate the embedded flaws in human behavior that can produce decisions that lead to under-optimized, inefficient, ineffective, or non-competitive business performance that wastes capital and resources. This punctuates why unbiased, scientific AI and analytics inputs are essential to minimize or eliminate group bias and contribute to improved business performance.
Individual Bias in Decision-Making
Individuals think in System 1 (thinking fast),