Attachment Theory and Research. Группа авторов. Читать онлайн. Newlib. NEWLIB.NET

Автор: Группа авторов
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Серия:
Жанр произведения: Социальная психология
Год издания: 0
isbn: 9781119657903
Скачать книгу
in any restrictive usage. First, there may be several different processes usually held in reserve: how many and which ones have to be mobilised before the disturbance can be classified as stressful? Secondly, some of these reserve processes may be mobilised in minimal degree even in routine responses to homeostatic disturbance. Thus, it is unlikely that any clear line of demarcation can be drawn between a routine disturbance and one of any degree beyond the routine.

      It has been noted that a homeostatic disturbance can occasionally be of a degree that is not fully restored and so causes development to change; but that the change process can be in a direction of advantage to the organism. Such disturbances are habitually referred to as stressful, but not, of course, as traumatic. Indeed stress having this type of effect is sometimes referred to as ‘strengthening experience’. In practice, however, it is often difficult to know what the balance of long‐term effect of a major stressor will prove to be and so whether the stress produced is to be judged as traumatic or strengthening.

      In what follows the terms ‘stress’ and ‘stressors’ are used fairly broadly. The varying degrees of disturbance and the varying outcomes are indicated by adjectives such as ‘routine’, ‘major’, ‘intense’, ‘strengthening’, and ‘traumatic’.

      [When] behavioural systems are under load, irrespective of which category of homeostasis being disturbed, it seems to be current practice to refer to the individual’s condition as one of psychological (or emotional) stress. We consider the behavioural systems engaged in restoring steady states within each category of homeostasis in turn, starting with the simplest cases.

      Any disturbance of personal‐environmental homeostasis is responded to by attempts by behavioural means to restore the steady state disturbed. Loss of a loved figure is responded to by search; a damaged home by attempts to rebuild it. As long as attempts to restore to state continue, the individual’s condition is habitually referenced to as one of psychological or emotional stress. Once success is achieved, stress ceases and relief is felt. Should the attempts fail and finally be abandoned on the other hand, stress also ceases. It is then replaced by despair and depression.

      An analogous set of responses and sequences follow any disturbance of ecological homeostasis. Thus, reduction of oxygen pressure elicits attempts to escape into fresh air; a large rise or fall in temperature elicits attempts to find a more equable ambient temperature; an environment lacking water of food elicits attempts to find a more congenial habitat. Insofar as it is again behavioural systems that are under load the condition is likely to be references to as one of psychological and emotional stress.

      The position regarding disturbances of morphological and physiological homeostasis is a little more complicated because, in the regulation of steady states of those sorts, two types of regulatory systems are called upon, physiological as well as behavioural. The term ‘physiological stress’, it should be noted, however, is habitually used to refer only to the load placed upon the relevant physiological systems. Insofar as a load may also be placed on relevant behavioural systems, the condition is referred to as one not of physiological but of psychological (or emotional) stress. An illustration is as follows: a person is suffering from incipient heat stroke in a desert. Physiological systems to reduce temperature are active and under heavy load. His condition would, further, be referred to as one of physiological stress. Yet simultaneously he might be struggling to escape the sun by climbing to a cave half‐way up a cliff, but be finding the feat almost beyond him. His mental state might then be referenced to as one of psychological (or emotional) stress.

      The position regarding disturbances of representational homeostasis is also a little complicated because, here again, two types of regulatory systems are called upon: psychological systems as well as behavioural ones. Current usage, it appears, is to refer to the condition as one of psychological (or emotional) stress when there is any disturbance of homeostasis, irrespective of which type of regulatory systems is relied upon. Let us consider the two types of systems successively.

      An alternative way of maintaining representational homeostasis is, by appropriate behaviour, to select the information likely to reach us so that it accords with our present working models. Whatever we suspect of being incompatible is almost automatically avoided, and a substantial effort may have to be made to give it attention. Much of this selection is achieved behaviourally by selecting what we read and listen to, though, as already indicated, much censoring is done also after our sense‐organs have been exposed to the unwanted information. Should a person be forced against his wish to listen to incompatible views, as in a brain‐washing session, the experience would clearly be regarded as stressful; and his condition would be described as one of psychological (or emotional) stress.

      To sum up, the terms ‘physiological stress’ and ‘psychological (or emotional) stress’ are used today respectively to refer, not to a disturbance of any particular category of homeostasis, but to the type of regulatory system that is called upon to restore the disturbance. Whenever physiological systems are activated the organism is (or may be) described as undergoing physiological stress; whenever either behavioural or psychological systems are activated the organism is (or may be) described as undergoing psychological (or emotional) stress.

      Thus far, the discussion has considered only actual disturbances of one or another category of homeostasis. In examining problems of psychological stress and anxiety, however, it is situations of threatened disturbance that are often of special importance.

      A stressor has been defined as any change in the internal state or external environment that disturbs a steady state and thereby elicits regulatory processes that have as their predictable outcome restoration of whatever steady state has been disturbed. Living organisms, however, usually do not wait for a stressor to act. More often than not they are forewarned and take one or another type of action the consequences of which are either to avoid the stressor’s striking or else to mitigate or cancel its effects.

      It is evident that, in the development of an animal’s capacity to take appropriate avoiding action, learning plays a major role. Every experiment in which an animal suffers punishment if it fails (e.g. when the buzzer goes it must press the right bar, or must turns down the right branch of a maze) is concerned with such learning. Nevertheless, there is good evidence that, in addition to all the cues portending trouble that are learned, there are other sorts of cues that lead animals to withdraw but that do not have to be learned. The strong tendency to avoid anything strange, which appears to be universal in birds and mammals, is one of the best investigated examples. The response of birds to the alarm call of their own species, and often to those of relative species, is another.

      That many of