Marleena Stolp from the University of Jyväskylä in Finland spent six weeks watching the theatre production, recording the children’s conversations and then analysing them.12 She soon found one topic that predominated – money. The children knew they were creating something with a market value; they discussed ticket prices and the possibility of filming the performance in order to sell the DVD in shops. They were only six years old, but far from viewing the play as simply an entertaining experience, they were already thinking about how to market and monetise it. There was no doubt that they loved the idea of making money. They even discussed how to select a ticket price that people would be prepared to pay, well aware that the market would not allow them to overcharge and that they risked having no paying audience if they did.
So these children already had some comprehension of money, pricing and the idea of the market. Where does this understanding of the value of money come from?
SAVING FOR A LUTE
In a study conducted in Hong Kong, a group of five- and six-year-olds were given the word ‘money’ and asked to free associate. They had plenty to say on the subject. Not surprisingly, they mainly associated it with the ability to buy things they wanted (similar studies in the US and Europe have found the same). They didn’t tend to have views on the virtue or otherwise of money.
Which is not the case with adults. When the same researchers gave adults questionnaires about whether money was good or bad, different groups took different stances. Students in particular had negative views about it. They believed it to be less good, less interesting and, strikingly, less powerful than business people did.13 By contrast the young children hadn’t developed ethical positions on money. It was just there, and they knew it was something desirable and useful, something you wanted to have, ready to spend. The notion of saving is something children learn about and appreciate even when they’re quite little. That said, before going to school, when children do save, the main motivation is usually the pleasure of collecting money, piling it up and counting it. It’s only as children get a bit older that they begin to save for a particular item they want to buy.
In my case, the cherished item was, rather bizarrely, a lute. I’d seen one at a craft fair at Hatfield House, the Tudor mansion in Hertfordshire where Elizabeth I was supposedly sitting under an oak tree in 1558 when she was given the news that she was to become queen. More than half a millennium later I became determined to save up enough to buy a lute. In order to track my progress, I carefully drew one of those fundraising thermometers. I also opened a special savings account at my favorite building society. I was extraordinarily, and rather sweetly, tenacious about my saving. After five years I had accumulated £187. Which was a good effort, but not nearly enough loot to buy a lute. The one I had my eye on cost £1,400.
Still, if I hadn’t been so set on the unobtainable lute I might never have saved that much. By the end of the five years, I was at the age where children realise that saving is useful more generally. Money doesn’t just have to be earmarked for a particular purchase. It represents choice. It’s worth having even when you’re not sure what you want to buy with it. Some time in the future you’ll want or need something, and if you have savings you might be able to afford that something.
Sadly, I rather frittered my lute money away. Having grown out of my Elizabethan phase, I spent my savings on records by Billy Idol, The Sisters of Mercy and U2. (Though, thinking about it, vinyl of that vintage might not be such a bad investment after all.)
This illustrates how hard it is to define ‘good’ saving among children. You could measure their saving as a proportion of any money they have received over a certain period of time, whether that’s regular pocket money or birthday money from their grandparents. But is saving up some of that money for several months or years and then buying an expensive toy really worthy of praise? If an adult did something similar – a fifty-year-old man spending all his savings on a pricey motorbike, for example – we might see him as extravagant. He might have saved up for some time, but then he blew the cash all in one go.
But then is it really better to save a little less for shorter periods and to spend that money on, say, books? If you really like books, this sort of saving is really just another form of indulgence. And what of saving a large sum with no great object in mind? It might seem responsible. You never know when you might need the money. But in an adult, such saving might seem stingy or even miserly.
Taking everything into account, adults generally applaud the regular saving of a proportion of income. We need to save in order to afford a deposit on a home. It’s a form of insurance for spells of unemployment or a bout of illness. And of course, we have to think about retirement. I’ll come back to how adults can trick themselves into saving more in Chapter 13. But children struggle with the concept of a ‘rainy day’ – the umbrella in these situations is provided by their parents, their very own ‘nanny state’. And the self-restraint that saving requires is a real trial for children who live much more in the moment.
This was nicely demonstrated in a study that created a so-called ‘play economy’. 14 Each child was told that to start them off they had an imaginary bank account containing 30 tokens. Then they were told that time in this imagined world was speeded up. Each ‘day’ lasted just 10 minutes and every ‘day’ they’d be given an additional 10 tokens. So, for instance, after the first hour – ‘6 days’ – if they’d spent none of the tokens in the meantime, they would have ‘saved’ 90 tokens.
Next, the children were shown around a set of rooms. Some activities were free. Others cost tokens. In the library, there was no charge for reading books, but they had to pay if they wanted to watch a film. In the room next door, video games attracted a charge, as did items in the café and the sweetshop, but borrowing pencils and paper for drawing was free. The decisions the children made about their spending would affect their activity in the final room – the toyshop – where they could buy real toys to take home, but only if a child still had 70 tokens left. You can see the excruciating calculations the children had to make. In order to get a toy in the toyshop, they would have to spend time, but very little money in the different rooms. It would mean forgoing computer games, food, drink and sweets for 40 minutes in order to accumulate those 70 tokens. They would be left with nothing to do but boring old reading or drawing.
Children tend to take experiments like this very seriously, but find them hard. Part of the reason is that for children such tests involve real sacrifice. This was demonstrated by Walter Mischel, the psychologist who invented the famous marshmallow test.15 As you may know, the marshmallow test offers children the choice between eating one marshmallow straight away or waiting 10 minutes to get two. An adult taking part would know they could show restraint during the test because they could always buy a whole bagful of marshmallows on the way home if they felt like it. The small child has no such get-out.
The children in the ‘play economy’ faced the same struggles, and very few had the willpower to save up enough tokens for a toy, however much they wanted it. They had already learnt that savings were a good thing, but when faced with more immediate temptations in the other rooms, they couldn’t restrain themselves. By the end of the experiment, only half the children had saved enough tokens for a toy and a quarter hadn’t saved any tokens at all. For those who worked out quite early on they were going to be a long way short of being able to ‘buy’ the toy, their overall behaviour was actually very rational. After all, they couldn’t take their play savings out of the play economy. Certainly they felt their ‘savings’ were money lost, rather than something useful.
BANKS, SHOPKEEPERS, ROBBERS AND TOOTH FAIRIES
Back in the 1980s, the influential Italian psychologists Anna Berti and Anna Bombi followed a group of three- to eight-year-olds in order to track how their ideas about money changed as they grew up.
What the two psychologists found was