The Science of Reading. Группа авторов. Читать онлайн. Newlib. NEWLIB.NET

Автор: Группа авторов
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Серия:
Жанр произведения: Языкознание
Год издания: 0
isbn: 9781119705130
Скачать книгу
L., Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Collateralization of Broca’s area and the visual word form area in left‐handers: fMRI evidence. Brain and Language, 122, 171–178. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.11.004.

      76 van Orden, G. C. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & Cognition, 15(3), 181–198. doi: 10.3758/bf03197716.

      77 van Wijnendaele, I., & Brysbaert, M. (2002). Visual word recognition in bilinguals: Phonological priming from the second to the first language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(3), 616–627. doi: 10.1037/0096‐1523.28.3.616.

      78 Vasilev, M. R., Yates, M., & Slattery, T. J. (2019). Do readers integrate phonological codes across saccades? A Bayesian meta‐analysis and a survey of the unpublished literature. Journal of Cognition, 2(1), 43. doi: 10.5334/joc.87.

      79 Vilhauer, R. (2016). Inner reading voices: An overlooked form of inner speech. Psychosis, 8(1), 37–47. doi: 10.1080/17522439.2015.1028972.

      80 Vilhauer, R. P. (2017). Characteristics of inner reading voices. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 58(4), 269–274. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12368.

      81 Ward, N. (1998). Artificial intelligence and other approaches to speech understanding: Reflection on methodology. Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence, 10, 487–493. doi: 10.1080/095281398146716.

      82 Whitney, C. (2001). How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(2), 221–243. doi: 10.3758/bf03196158.

      83 Woollams, A. M., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Madrid, G., & Patterson, K. E. (2016). Do you read how I read? Systematic individual differences in semantic reliance amongst normal readers. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1757. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01757.

      84 Wright, G., Sherman, R., & Jones, T. B. (2004). Are silent reading behaviors of first graders really silent? The Reading Teacher, 57(6), 546–553.

      85 Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G. (2010). Chinese–English bilinguals reading English hear Chinese. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(22), 7646–7651. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1602‐10.2010.

      86 Xu, B., & Perfetti, C. A. (1999). Nonstrategic subjective threshold effects in phonemic masking. Memory & Cognition, 27(1), 26–36. doi: 10.3758/bf03201210.

      87 Zhang, S., & Perfetti, C. A. (1993). The tongue‐twister effect in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(5), 1082–1093. doi: 10.1037/0278‐7393.19.5.1082.

      88 Zhao, R., Fan, R., Liu, M., Wang, X., & Yang, J. (2017). Rethinking the function of brain regions for reading Chinese characters in a meta‐analysis of fMRI studies. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 44, 120–133. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2017.04.001.

      89 Zhou, H., Chen, B., Yang, M., & Dunlap, S. (2010). Language nonselective access to phonological representations: Evidence from Chinese–English bilinguals. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(10), 2051–2066. doi: 10.1080/17470211003718705.

      90 Zhou, P., & Christianson, K. (2016). I “hear” what you're “saying”: Auditory perceptual simulation, reading speed, and reading comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(5), 972–995. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1018282.

      91 Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 3–29. doi: 10.1037/0033‐2909.131.1.3.

      92 Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., & Zorzi, M. (2014). Modelling reading development through phonological decoding and self‐teaching: Implications for dyslexia. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634), 20120397. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0397.

      93 Ziegler, J. C., Petrova, A., & Ferrand, L. (2008). Feedback consistency effects in visual and auditory word recognition: Where do we stand after more than a decade? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(3), 643–661. doi: 10.1037/0278‐7393.34.3.643.

      Kathleen Rastle

      The past 50 years of research on visual word recognition has been dominated by the view that the primary challenge of reading is to decode the printed word to a spoken language representation. Thousands of articles have been devoted to understanding how skilled readers compute sound‐based (phonological) representations from printed words (e.g., Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler, 2001; Plaut, McClelland, Seidenberg & Patterson, 1996), how phonological decoding constrains word identification (e.g., Lukatela & Turvey, 1994), the time‐course of phonological decoding (e.g., Rastle & Brysbaert, 2006; Rayner et al., 1995), and whether it is obligatory (e.g., Frost, 1998). Likewise, research on learning to read has focused on how phonological decoding ability influences reading success (e.g., Melby‐ Lervåg, Lyster, & Hulme, 2012), how inconsistency in the relationship between spellings and sounds affects learning to read (e.g., Seymour, Aro & Erskine, 2003), and how children should be taught to relate visual symbols to sounds (see e.g., Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018). This body of research has demonstrated unambiguously that the computation of phonological representations plays a vital role in skilled reading and learning to read (see Brysbaert, this volume).