The Science of Reading. Группа авторов. Читать онлайн. Newlib. NEWLIB.NET

Автор: Группа авторов
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Серия:
Жанр произведения: Языкознание
Год издания: 0
isbn: 9781119705130
Скачать книгу
Topics in Cognitive Science, 5(1), 132–172. doi: 10.1111/tops.12010.

      30 Glushko, R. J. (1979). The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5(4), 674–691. doi: 10.1037/0096‐1523.5.4.674.

      31 Gordon, J. K., & Dell, G. S. (2003). Learning to divide the labor: An account of deficits in light and heavy verb production. Cognitive Science, 27(1), 1–40. doi: 10.1016/S0364‐0213(02)00111‐8.

      32 Graves, W. W., Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Humphries, C., Stengel, B. C., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2014). Anatomy is strategy: Skilled reading differences associated with structural connectivity differences in the reading network. Brain and Language, 133, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.03.005.

      33 Griffiths, T. L., Chater, N., Kemp, C., Perfors, A., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2010). Probabilistic models of cognition: Exploring representations and inductive biases. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(8), 357–364. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.004.

      34 Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Reading acquisition, phonology, and dyslexia: Insights from a connectionist model. Psychological Review, 106, 491–528. doi: 10.1037/0033‐295x.106.3.491.

      35 Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in reading: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111(3), 662–720. doi: 10.1037/0033‐295X.111.3.662.

      36 Hoffman, P., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Woollams, A. M. (2015). Triangulation of the neurocomputational architecture underpinning reading aloud. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(28), E3719–E3728. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1502032112.

      37 Jared, D. (1997). Spelling‐sound consistency affects the naming of high‐frequency words. Journal of Memory and Language, 36(4), 505–529. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1997.2496.

      38 Jared, D. (2002). Spelling‐sound consistency and regularity effects in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(4), 723–750. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2001.2827.

      39 Jared, D., McRae, K., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1990). The basis of consistency effects in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29(6), 687–715. doi: 10.1016/0749‐596X(90)90044‐Z.

      40 Joanisse, M. F., & McClelland, J. L. (2015). Connectionist perspectives on language learning, representation and processing. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6(3), 235–247. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1340.

      41 Juliano, C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1994). A constraint‐based lexicalist account of the subject/object attachment preference. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 23(6), 459–471. doi: 10.1007/BF02146685.

      42 Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

      43 Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (2001). Relationships between sounds and letters in English monosyllables. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(4), 592–617. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2745.

      44 Kumaran, D., Hassabis, D., & McClelland, J. L. (2016). What learning systems do intelligent agents need? Complementary learning systems theory updated. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(7), 512–534. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.004.

      45 Lambon Ralph, M. A., Jefferies, E., Patterson, K., & Rogers, T. T. (2017). The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(1), 42–55. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.150.

      46 MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101(4), 676–703. doi: 10.1037/0033‐295x.101.4.676.

      47 Marshall, J. C., & Newcombe, F. (1973). Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2(3), 175–199. doi: 10.1007/BF01067101.

      48 Mason, M. (1978). The role of spatial redundancy in grapheme recognition: Perception or inference? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4(4), 662–673. doi: 10.1037/0096‐1523.4.4.662.

      49 New, B., Ferrand, L., Pallier, C., & Brysbaert, M. (2006). Reexamining the word length effect in visual word recognition: new evidence from the English Lexicon Project. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 45–52. doi: 10.3758/bf03193811.

      50 Paap, K. R., & Noel, R. W. (1991). Dual‐route models of print to sound: Still a good horse race. Psychological Research, 53(1), 13–24. doi: 10.1007/BF00867328.

      51 Patterson, K., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (1999). Selective disorders of reading? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9(2), 235–239. doi: 10.1016/s0959‐4388(99)80033‐6.

      52 Pearlmutter, N. J., & MacDonald, M. C. (1995). Individual differences and probabilistic constraints in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(4), 521–542. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1995.1024.

      53 Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: The CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychological Review, 114(2), 273–315. doi: 10.1037/0033‐295X.114.2.273.

      54 Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Zorzi, M. (2010). Beyond single syllables: Large‐scale modeling of reading aloud with the Connectionist Dual Process (CDP++) model. Cognitive Psychology, 61(2), 106–151. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.04.001.

      55 Perry, C., Zorzi, M., & Ziegler, J. C. (2019). Understanding dyslexia through personalized large‐scale computational models. Psychological Science, 30(3), 386–395. doi: 10.1177/0956797618823540.

      56 Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. William Morrow & Co.

      57 Plaut, D. C., & Kello, C. T. (1999). The emergence of phonology from the interplay of speech comprehension and production: A distributed connectionist approach. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), The emergence of language (pp. 381–415). Erlbaum.

      58 Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi‐regular domains. Psychological Review, 103(1), 56–115. doi: 10.1037/0033‐295x.103.1.56.

      59 Pritchard, S. C., Coltheart, M., Marinus, E., & Castles, A. (2018). A computational model of the self‐teaching hypothesis based on the dual‐route cascaded model of reading. Cognitive Science, 42(3), 722–770. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12571.

      60 Pritchard, S. C., Coltheart, M., Palethorpe, S., & Castles, A. (2012). Nonword reading: Comparing dual‐route cascaded and connectionist dual‐process models with human data. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1268–1288. doi: 10.1037/a0026703.

      61 Rastle, K. (2016). Visual word recognition. In G. Hickok & S. L. Small (Eds.), Neurobiology of language (pp. 255–264). Academic Press. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198568971.013.0005.

      62 Rastle, K., & Coltheart, M. (1999). Serial and strategic effects in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25. (2), 482–503. doi: 10.1037/0096‐1523.25.2.482.

      63 Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In J. L. McClelland, D. E. Rumelhart, & the PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition. Volume 2 (pp. 216–271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

      64 Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L., & PDP Research Group (Eds.), (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Volume 1. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

      65 Seidenberg, M. S. (1985). The time course of phonological code activation in two writing systems. Cognition, 19(1), 1–30. doi: 10.1016/0010‐0277(85)90029‐0.

      66 Seidenberg, M. S. (1988). Cognitive neuropsychology and language: The state of the art. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5(4), 403–426. doi: 10.1080/02643298808253267.

      67 Seidenberg, M. S. (1992). Beyond orthographic depth: Equitable division of labor. In R. Frost and L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 85–118). Elsevier.

      68 Seidenberg, M. S. (2017). Language