The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. Группа авторов. Читать онлайн. Newlib. NEWLIB.NET

Автор: Группа авторов
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Серия:
Жанр произведения: Общая психология
Год издания: 0
isbn: 9781119678991
Скачать книгу
a male and female ethnographer simultaneously went to each community and carried out systematic observation and data collection, based on instructions from a single field manual, on childrearing behavior and socialization techniques. The Six Cultures studies allowed for certain conclusions to be drawn about the interplay between cultural variations in childrearing, the later personality of the child, and wider aspects of society. One of the most important of these conclusions was the level of complexity of the society; the Six Cultures studies suggested that children in complex societies tended to be less nurturant and more egotistical than those in simple societies. They found that girls, in all six cultures, were more nurturant than boys and that children of nuclear families were generally low on aggression and high on sociable interaction while the opposite was true for children of polygamous households (Whiting, 1977). The Six Cultures studies provide a wealth of detail about children’s lives, how they were treated, and their place in the life‐cycle. There were problems with the implementation of some of the psychological tests, as both Whiting (1977) and LeVine (2007) point out, and some of the methods were later discounted as unreliable. However, the data from this project are still used and developed and it has remained one of anthropology’s most comprehensive studies of children’s social development (Whiting & Edwards, 1988). Its influence can still be seen in the work of anthropologists such as Sara Harkness, whose account of culture and social development was a notable feature of this book’s first edition (Harkness, 2002). As LeVine (2003, pp. 202–203) concludes of these studies:

      The importance of these findings does not depend on an assumption that the child behavior patterns observed are fixed psychological dispositions that will maintain themselves regardless of environmental support. Rather, the findings indicate that the direction of child development, and the behavioral contexts of early experience, vary by culture according to adult standards of conduct. They also show that children of different cultures acquire different interpersonal skills and strategies, differing rules for emotional expression, and differing standards by which to judge their own behavior.

      Robert LeVine has analyzed childrearing practices and child socialization extensively in the decades since he worked with John Whiting as one of the Six Cultures researchers, and he remains at the forefront of research on children and their social development. He has strongly argued that studying social development cross‐culturally is central to studying children within anthropology, and such work forms the backbone of an anthropology of childhood. His work has illuminated the interplay of cultural belief and social development and shown how childrearing practices are rational, adaptive processes that enable children to grow up effectively, understanding the norms of their society.

      Much of LeVine’s work is based on comparisons of middle‐class mothers in the United States with those of mothers in rural communities in Kenya such as the Gusii. He identifies two approaches – pediatric and pedagogic – which may help explain why parents prioritize different goals in different circumstances and why social development in young children is not a priority for all parents in all cases. He argues that the pediatric approach, which is customary in parts of Africa, focuses on protection and survival in the early years, and the pedagogic approach practiced in the United States, is more concerned with teaching young children behavioral competencies. In African societies, where infant mortality is high, and the early years of life most dangerous, mothers are likely to keep their children in very close contact with them, carry them everywhere, and breast‐feed them for up to 2 years. They will feed them on demand but generally do not treat babies as emotionally responsive individuals with whom they should make eye contact, talk to, or be concerned about their behavioral development (LeVine, 1977). This is not to claim that they are uninterested in their long‐term development, or have not made explicit plans for events later on in life such as betrothal or initiation, but when children are very young, parents are more concerned with their physical survival rather than with their social development. In contrast, many middle‐class mothers in the United States (where infant mortality is low) may take the survival of children for granted, and so are more likely to devote time and energy to shaping how their children behave and get on socially and academically.

      Continuing the tradition and interdisciplinarity of comparative research on children’s social development, and also drawing on data from the HRAF, David Lancy’s work has focused on children’s learning and the role it plays in social development and socialization (Lancy, 2014). He is particularly interested in middle childhood as a time when children learn from their peers as much (or even more) than from their parents or other adults and when, crucially, they are rarely directly “taught” skills or behaviors but are nevertheless expected to learn and master particular tasks (Lancy et al., 2010). He refers to these key skills and behaviors as the “chore curriculum”: the tasks that children need to learn (depending on the social expectations of gender and their growing physical and cognitive competence) in order to function effectively in any society (Lancy, 2012). This curriculum is not learned in schools, or from adults, but primarily through interactions with other children, thereby blending socialization, learning, play, and work. Lancy (1977, p. 87) describes children’s social development among Kpelle blacksmiths in Liberia where he conducted fieldwork in the 1970s:

      I didn’t find that play has no relationship to work. They are, to use a favourite anthropological term, “integrated” … make believe play seems to be one step in an alternatively collapsing and expanding process. A child of three spends hours observing a blacksmith at work. A child of four brings his stick down on a rock repeatedly and says he is a blacksmith. A child of eight weaves with his friends an elaborate reconstruction of the blacksmith’s craft, all in make‐believe. The child of ten is a blacksmith’s helper in reality; he fetches wood for the forge and no more. At twelve he begins learning the actual skills of smithing, adding a new one every few months or so. At eighteen he is a full‐fledged blacksmith with his own forge. Parallel patterns can be observed for virtually every class of work.

      He has since argued that this pattern can be more widely observed and that children learn social and economic competence within their communities through observation of their peers. The chore curriculum is a form of learning and social development which takes place outside formal educational settings, or directed teaching, by parents, and Lancy emphasizes the impossibility of separating out social development from economic, cultural, and educational development.