The Behavior of Animals. Группа авторов. Читать онлайн. Newlib. NEWLIB.NET

Автор: Группа авторов
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Серия:
Жанр произведения: Биология
Год издания: 0
isbn: 9781119109525
Скачать книгу
are not merely triggered by objects in a reflexive or habitual way, but arise from a meaningful interpretation of an object by an individual” (p. 317). It considers the identification of emotional quality (its meaning) to require appraisal of the object and situation; it is the meaning that then leads to internal state changes: we are afraid of the bear when we see it (we appraise the situation) and then we become aroused and flee. Although Schachter & Singer use the word “appraisal” in their theory, Gendron & Barrett consider their theory closer in content to the psychological construction approach.

      The psychological construction approach posits that emotions are constructed out of more basic psychological ingredients that are not themselves specific to emotion. Two such basic components were proposed by Russell (2003): core affect and affective quality. “Core affect is that neurophysiological state consciously accessible as the simplest raw (nonreflective) feelings evident in moods and emotions” (p. 148). It is a single integral blend of two dimensions: pleasure-displeasure (which can range from elation to agony) and activation-deactivation (which can range from frenetic excitement to sleep). The feeling is an assessment of one’s current condition. Affective quality is a property of the stimulus: its capacity to change core affect. Perception of affective quality together with core affect allows a person to construct the emotion.

      Barrett (2013) believes that psychological construction constitutes a paradigm for the scientific study of emotion that is different from the “faculty” psychology paradigm of the basic emotion and appraisal approaches. She points to three principles of psychological construction that define this difference: the principles of variation, of core systems, and of emergentism and holism. I will not discuss these principles here, but I will say that in many respects the details of these principles bear a striking resemblance to many of the ideas originally expressed by William James (and acknowledged by Barrett and Russell). Whether these ideas will actually change the way researchers on emotional issues behave, remains to be seen. Barrett also calls psychological construction the Darwinian approach to the science of emotion, primarily because pre-Darwin, species were considered fixed whereas post-Darwin, the variety within species could be exploited by natural selection and lead to new kinds. Although there are some similarities between the two approaches, I doubt that most biologists would be impressed with the analogy.

      In considering these various approaches to the study of emotion, I would propose that it is the activated behavior system that determines the quality of the emotion. The study of emotion then becomes the study of what behavior systems exist in any organism, what motivational factors activate them, and how they are expressed. Emotions are the subjective aspect of strongly activated behavior systems. A corollary of this conceptualization is that the felt emotion becomes an epiphenomenon: like the whistle of the steam engine, it has no causal significance—which is, of course, consonant with James’ viewpoint. Much of the research on emotion in the past 50 years can be understood in these terms.

      Nonhuman Emotion

      Figure 3.8 Facial expressions of fear and aggression in dogs. Explanation in text. (From Lorenz 1966).

      Most studies of animal emotion are directed to understanding human emotion, but the rise of interest in animal welfare has led many investigators to study animal emotion per se. In the context of welfare, it is crucial to discover what makes an animal ‘feel good’ (or, at least, not suffer). However, feelings are subjective and we can never know what an animal feels (see Panksepp 2010 and Dawkins 2015 for recent discussions of animal consciousness). M. Dawkins (2008) suggests that a scientific study of animal suffering and welfare can be based on answers to two questions: Will the situation improve animal health? And, will it give animals something they want? The answer to the second question can be determined by discovering what the animal finds positively and negatively reinforcing (what they want and do not want) in a learning situation. Even here, however, a difference between “wanting” and “liking” (Berridge 2004) makes interpretation of the results not straightforward (an animal may like something, but not want it at this moment). Nonetheless, Dawkins’ approach seems the most reasonable proposal to date. Theoretically, Mendl et al. (2010) have proposed a framework that integrates the discrete emotion approach (i.e., the basic emotion approach above) with the dimensional approach (i.e., the psychological construction approach above) for the study of animal emotion and mood. The cognitive aspects of the dimensional approach allow one to experimentally dissociate “liking” something from currently