59 Pascalis, O., De Haan, M. & Nelson, C. A. 2002. Is face processing species-specific during the first year of life? Science, 296, 1321–1323.
60 Perret, D.I. & Rolls, E.T. 1983. Neural mechanisms underlying the visual analysis of faces. In J.-P. Ewert, R. R. Capranica, & D. J. Ingle (eds.), Advances in vertebrate neuroethology, pp. 543–566. New York: Plenum Press.
61 Prater, C.M., Harris, B.N., & Carr, J.A. 2020. Tectal CRFR1 receptor involvement in avoidance and approach behaviors in the South African clawed frog, Xenopus laevis. Hormones and Behavior, 120, 104707. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104707.
62 Prete, F.R. (ed.). 2004. Complex worlds from simpler nervous systems. Cambridge, MA: MTT Press.
63 Reddipogu, A., Maxwell, G. & MacLeod, C. 2002. An innovative neural network based on the toad’s visual system. Proceedings of ACIVS, Advanced Concepts for Intelligent Vision Systems, Ghent, Belgium.
64 Rowland, W.J. 1989. Mate choice and the supemormality effect in female sticklebacks (Gaster-osteus aculeatus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 24, 433–438.
65 Ryan, M.J. & Rand, A.S. 1995. Female responses to ancestral advertisement calls in the tungara frog. Science, 269, 390–392.
66 Schleidt, W. 1961. Über die Auslösung der Flucht vor Raubvögeln bei Truthühnern. Naturwissenschaften, 48, 141–142.
67 Schürg-Pfeiffer, E., Spreckelsen, C. & Ewert, J.-P. 1993. Temporal discharge patterns of tectal and medullary neurons chronically recorded during snapping toward prey in toads Bufo spinosus. Journal Comparative Physiology A, 173, 363–376.
68 Seitz, A. 1940. Die paarbildung bei einigen cichliden. Zeitschrift Tierpsychologie, 4, 40–84.
69 Singer, W. 1995. Development and plasticity of cortical processing architectures. Science, 270, 758–764.
70 Spreckelsen, C., Schürg-Pfeiffer, E. & Ewert, J.-P. 1995. Responses of retinal and tectal neurons in non-paralyzed toads Bufo and B. marinus to the real size versus angular size of objects moved at variable distance. Neuroscience Letters, 184, 105–108.
71 Suga, N. 1990. Biosonar and neural computation in bats. Scientific American, 262, 60–68.
72 Ter Polkwijk, J.J. & Tinbergen, N. 1937. Eine reizbiologische Analyse einigen Verhaltensweisen von Gasterosteus aculeatus. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 1, 194–200.
73 Tinbergen, N. 1951. The Study of Instinct. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Reissued in 1989 by Oxford University Press, New York.
74 Tinbergen, N. & Kuenen, D.J. 1939. Über die auslösenden und die richtunggebenden Reizsituationen der Sperrbewegung von jungen Drosseln (Turdus m. merula L. und T. e. ericetorum Turton). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 3, 37-60.
75 Udin, S. 1977. Rearrangements of the retinotectal projection in Rana pipiens after unilateral caudal half-tectum ablation. Journal Comparative Neurology, 173(3), 561–582.
76 Ungerleider, L.G. & Mishkin, M. 1982. Two cortical visual systems. In D.J. Ingle, M.A. Goodale & R.J.W. Mansfield (eds.), Analysis of visual behavior, pp. 549–586. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
77 Valenza, E., Simion, F., Cassia, V.M. & Umilta, C. 1996. Face preference at birth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 892–903.
78 Von Uexkiill, J. 1921. Umwelt und Innenwelt der Tiere. Berlin: Springer.
79 Wiersma, C.A.G. & Ikeda, K. 1964. Interneurons commanding swimmeret movements in the crayfish, Procambarus clarkii (Girard). Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, 12, 509–525.
80 Wiltschko, W. & Wiltschko, R. 2002. Magnetic compass orientation in birds and its physiological basis. Naturwissenschaften, 89, 445–452.
81 Yoshida, N. 2016. From retina to behavior: prey-predator recognition by convolutional neural networks and their modulation by classical conditioning. Adaptive Behavior, 1–23. doi:10.1177/1059712316650265.
82 Zupanc, G.H. 2019. Behavioral neurobiology. An integrative approach, 3rd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
3 motivation and emotion
JERRY A. HOGAN
INTRODUCTION
The word motivate means “to cause to move,” and I will use the concept of motivation to refer to the study of the immediate causes of behavior: those factors responsible for the initiation, maintenance, and termination of behavior. Thus, motivation is another word for aspects of Tinbergen’s causal question (see Chapter 1). Causal factors for behavior include stimuli, hormones, and the intrinsic activity of the nervous system. How do these factors cause a female rat to behave maternally to her pups? Or a chicken to bathe in dust in the middle of the day? Or a male stickleback fish to stop responding sexually to receptive females? These are the types of questions asked in the first part of this chapter.
Motivated behavior often produces emotion, but the concept of emotion is problematic because there is no consensus about its definition. In the second part of this chapter I will analyze the concept of emotion as applied primarily to humans and conclude with a section on nonhuman emotion and its relation to animal welfare.
Behavior Systems
A major problem in the study of both motivation and emotion is that different authors use these concepts in different ways. The concept of a behavior system is useful in understanding many of these differences. I have proposed perceptual, central, and motor mechanisms as the basic structural units of behavior. These entities are viewed as corresponding to structures within the central nervous system. They consist of an arrangement of neurons (not necessarily localized) that acts independently of other such mechanisms. Perceptual mechanisms analyze incoming sensory information and solve the problem of stimulus recognition. An example is the releasing mechanism discussed in Chapter 2. The motor mechanisms are responsible for coordinating the neural output to the muscles, which results in recognizable patterns of movement. The central mechanisms coordinate the perceptual and motor mechanisms and also provide the basis for an animal’s mood or internal state. These units are called behavior mechanisms because their activation results in an event of behavioral interest: a particular perception, a specific motor pattern, or an identifiable internal state.
Behavior mechanisms can be connected with one another to form larger units called behavior systems, which correspond to the level of complexity indicated by feeding, sexual, and aggressive behavior (Baerends 1976; Hogan 2001). The organization of the connections among behavior mechanisms determines the nature of the behavior system. Thus, a behavior system can be considered a description of the structure of behavior. A pictorial representation of this concept is shown in Figure 3.1.
Figure 3.1 Conception of behavior systems. Stimuli from the external world are analyzed by perceptual mechanisms. Output from the perceptual mechanisms can be integrated by central mechanisms and/or channeled directly to motor mechanisms. The output of the motor mechanisms results in behavior. In this diagram, central mechanism I, perceptual mechanisms 1, 2, and 3, and motor mechanisms A, B, and C form one behavior system; central mechanism II, perceptual mechanisms 3, 4, and 5, and motor mechanisms C, D,and E form a second behavior system; 1-A, 2-B, and so on can also be considered less complex behavior systems. (From Hogan 1988).
Causal